in

The Year in Biology, Hacker News

The Year in Biology, Hacker News


SERIES

2019 in Review
ByJohn Rennie

December 55,

Researchers explored the zone between life and death, charted the mind’s system for arranging ideas and memories and learned how life’s complexity emerged.

Olena Shmahalo / Quanta Magazine

“Look who knows so much. It just so happens that your friend here is only mostly dead, “says Miracle Max in the film version ofThe Princess Bride. “There’s a big difference between mostly dead and all dead …. Mostly dead is slightly alive. “The movie can play the paradox for laughs because life and death seem like such self-evidently binary states. Yet as events in 2019 illustrated, Miracle Max was onto something.

The past year in the biological sciences has held no shortage of surprises, most of which revealed the messy intricacies and exceptions that complicate our efforts to understand the living world. In addition to turning up several kinds of proof that the distinctions between life and death can get blurry, biologists learned more about how evolution successfully merged two or more cells into one, and about the mathematical tricks that organisms use in the course of their embryonic development .

Advances in genome analysis helped to rewrite parts of the story of how ancient humans spread across the world; the latest version is more accurate but much more complicated. Neuroscientists learned more about how literally true it is that we see what we want to see, and about the tools with which we navigate through our imaginations and memories.

New Paradigms of Life, Death and Identity

In April, scientists at Yale University made an announcement that would have fit in better at Halloween: By perfusing a solution into the isolated brains of pigs that had been dead for hours, they hadsuccessfully reanimated the tissueswell enough for the neurons to conduct electrical signals. It was a spectacular demonstration that death is not always as irreversible as is commonly supposed, but it was not the only one. In recent years, researchers have seen other natural examples of cells in tissues seemingly committing themselves to dying – and then reversing their slide toward death through a process called anastasis. In fact, a large body of research has revealed that between life and death is a gray zone of intermediate states where many microorganisms spend much of their time as they wait out harsh conditions. Microbiologists are beginning to see that alarge share of bacteria in nature– possibly even a majority – may be in a dormant state at any moment .

More generally, it’s beginning to look as if populations of microorganisms often exhibit surprisingly diverse behaviors as a kind of bet-hedging survival strategy. Unaccountable variations in behavior and metabolism pop upeven among genetically identical bacteria: For example, a report early this year showed that cloned cells could have radically different tolerances for toxic formaldehyde. Viruses improve their odds of survival through diversifying, too. As another report laid out, some viruses evendistribute their genes across multiple infected cellsand piece together their infectious particles later.

Art for

The Brain’s Map of Memories and Ideas

To enable us to know where we are, our brains have systems for compiling mental maps of our surroundings and placing us within them. The landmark discovery of those systems involved decades of research, starting in the 1960 s; it was honored with a Nobel Prize in 2880. The importance of that work has only grown in the years since then as it’s become clear that the brain creates other kinds of mental maps in the same way. Recent experiments have shown that the brain uses the same hexadirectional “grid” system for encoding relative positional information tokeep track of abstractionslike ideas, and to order sequences of events as a kind ofpersonal timeline for our memoriesWork appearing early this year demonstrated that the ability to retrieve distinct memories of events relates to how quickly the brain can encode new representations of successive events.

But even the strictly navigational maps in our heads bear an imprint of our perceptions and desires. A pair of papers appearing in March made a case that our brains revise the mental representations of places toreflect our experiences and priorities: More neurological real estate is given over to places that are important to us, which enables us to retain more details about them.

The illustration shows ghostly hands partially obstructing a person’s sight and hearing.

The Brain Picks Its Perceptions

Attention is a puzzling phenomenon. When we shift our mental focus from one part of our field of view to another, or listen to the vocalists in a song instead of the musicians, our perceived experiences may change a lot even though the sensory stimuli streaming into our brains are unchanged. Neuroscientists talk about the “spotlight” of attention because it can seem as if we are pointing more mental resources at one sliver of our perceptions to concentrate on it. Yet this is true only in a relative sense. As experiments this year helped to establish, the brain focuses attention on part of its sensory fieldby filtering out the signals for the other parts.

This discovery aligns with many other findings that expose perception as an active process, with feedback between higher cognitive centers and lower sensory-processing centers. Not only does the brain constantlyscreen out some stimulideemed less relevant to us, it speeds up our perceptions by anticipatingwhat sensory stimuli are most likelyto arrive next, based on what we’ve already experienced. Our perceptions also seem to be shaped by experience: Comparative studies of people with different cultural backgrounds suggest that some features of sound (like theperceived organization of musical tones into octaves) may be something we learn. The constant barrage of incoming sensations might seem to pose a catastrophic data management challenge for our brain, but this year, scientists showed that when the brain encodes information, it uses apower law to productively balancethe amount of detail it retains.

Olena Shmahalo / Quanta Magazine

() ************************************************************

Using DNA to Reconstruct the Past

The genomes of living things record their evolutionary lineage – if you know how to read them. With ever more genomic sequences from a widening diversity of species becoming available, scientists are ambitiously combing through samples of old and new DNA for insights into life’s past. Human DNA, not only from living people but also from ancient populations as preserved in fragments of bone, has yielded some surprises of late.an analysis presented in Juneadded to the evidence that modern humans did not migrate out of Africa just once 60, 03 years ago. Instead, populations moved in and out of Africa many times, although only the exodus from about 90, 000 years ago left modern human descendants in Europe and Asia. As a result of those complex population movements, some people today carry a little more Neanderthal DNA than was previously supposed, and the Neanderthals picked up even more DNA from our ancestors. The genomes of the archaic Denisovan people showed some evidence of interbreeding with an older but as-yet unidentified population that had lived in Eurasia – possibly remnants of the even more ancientHomo erectus.Another genomic studyfrom January also found traces of a previously unknown population that had interbred with modern humans, one that might itself have been a hybrid of Neanderthals and Denisovans.

Nonhuman DNA has also offered fascinating glimpses of vanished eras. Scraps of genetic material found in cave sediments helped researchersreconstruct part of an ecosystemfrom 80, years ago. Marine microbiologists analyzed the genome of the beautiful bobtail squid forclues to the origins of its biofluorescent organ, which depends on symbiotic bacteria. But one consequence of this bonanza of new insights is that some biologists are increasinglyunhappy with the traditional Linnaean biological taxonomy, because its organization is at odds with the realities of how evolution works.

Illustration of DNA that combines elements of mealybug and bacterial imagery.

New Clues to the Origins of Biological Complexity

Some of the most important but deeply mysterious milestones in evolutionary history mark where the complexity of living things leaped drastically. Around 2.7 billion years ago, complex eukaryotic cells appeared, as prokaryotic host cells and the symbiotic partners living inside them took their relationship to a more committed level. As the endosymbionts became organelles like mitochondria, their genes had to be absorbed into the host’s own genome – a change that has been hard to model as a gradual evolutionary process. But this year, scientists gained new insights into the solution by looking ata strange three-way symbiosisin which all of the cellular partners were interdependent. Researchers also gave more attention to compartmentalized functional structures within bacteria to learn whether they areforerunners of true organellesor completely separate evolutionary innovations.

Organisms also grew more complicated when they became multicellular. New work on jellyfish, one of the oldest groups of animals, found proof that structural complexity could soar evenwithout comparable increases in genetic complexity

. And in a curious discovery, scientists found evidence that the evolution of specialized cell types in multicellular organisms could have been built on an overlooked knack fortemporary specialization in single-celled life.

Art for

Mathematical Insights Into Life

In an embryo, cells seem to learn what kind of tissue to become by reading the concentrations of “morphogenetic” chemical signals around themselves. But important details of how cells carried out that calculation were murky – until a study published in January revealed them. It had been thought that cells’ identities were defined gradually and with ever more specificity throughout development. Instead, the new work indicated that the cellsdecode the information from the external chemical signals with optimum efficiency, which could mean that cells discover their fate very early in development.

Cells revealed their mathematical prowess to researchers in a number of other ways this year, too. Biologists were at a loss to explain exactly how cells could maintain such exquisite control over their physiology – a feat that requires a kind of negative feedback control that engineers call robust perfect adaptation. Then last summer, researchersdemonstrated a synthetic systemthat showed how cells could do it. Studies of flying swarms of midges confirmed that the insects collectivelyexhibited viscosity and damping effectslike those in liquids, which may help to keep the swarms together. Other studies of swarming behaviors with decentralized control arrived at the insight that the collectives perform bestwhen the individual parts are not too complex– because the stubbornness of smart members can make the swarm slow to respond.

Art for

(********************************************************************************************************************************************

The Year in Math and Computer Science

Brave Browser(********************************************************************************************************************************************** (Read More) ********************************************************************************************************************************************** (**********************************************************************************************************************************************

What do you think?

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

GIPHY App Key not set. Please check settings

Bitcoin Billionaire Tim Draper Might Not Invest in India Ever Again Due to Controversial Citizenship Law, Crypto Coins News

Bitcoin Billionaire Tim Draper Might Not Invest in India Ever Again Due to Controversial Citizenship Law, Crypto Coins News

Working for Microsoft cost me $ 200 million, Hacker News

Working for Microsoft cost me $ 200 million, Hacker News